RESEARCH ARTICLE


Effects of Time Utilization on the Well-being of College Athletes



Jay C. Santos1, *, Michael Sagas2
1 Department of Psychology, Central Luzon State University, Science City of Muñoz, Nueva Ecija 3121, Philippines
2 Department of Sport Management, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL 32611, USA


Article Metrics

CrossRef Citations:
1
Total Statistics:

Full-Text HTML Views: 2231
Abstract HTML Views: 489
PDF Downloads: 419
ePub Downloads: 245
Total Views/Downloads: 3384
Unique Statistics:

Full-Text HTML Views: 1343
Abstract HTML Views: 297
PDF Downloads: 289
ePub Downloads: 188
Total Views/Downloads: 2117



Creative Commons License
© 2022 Santos and Sagas

open-access license: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Public License (CC-BY 4.0), a copy of which is available at: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode. This license permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

* Address correspondence to this author at the Department of Psychology, College of Arts and Social Sciences, Central Luzon State University, Science City of Muñoz, Nueva Ecija 3121, Philippines; E-mail: jcsantos@clsu.edu.ph


Abstract

Background:

Considering that college student-athletes spend a lot of time attending to academic and athletic tasks, their well-being can be compromised. Published literature on the impact of time demands on the well-being of college student-athletes is limited. Hence, the scholarly place of the current study is warranted.

Objective:

This research investigated the effects of academic time use and athletic time use on the subjective, emotional, and physical well-being of college student-athletes.

Methods:

A subset of the NCAA GOALS data, which included 5,042 student-athletes, was utilized. Two research questions were examined: (1) to what extent does academic time use predict subjective, emotional, and physical well-being? and (2) to what extent does athletic time use predict subjective, emotional, and physical well-being? and (2) to what extent does athletic time use predict subjective, emotional, and physical well-being? A series of two-step hierarchical regression analyses were performed. Race, gender, and NCAA division level were used as control variables.

Results and Discussion:

Results revealed that academic time use is a significant positive predictor of subjective well-being. Moreover, athletic time use is also a significant predictor of all well-being indicators. It positively predicted subjective well-being but negatively predicted emotional and physical well-being.

Conclusion:

Our findings showed that both academic and athletic time utilizations are significant predictors of subjective well-being but in varying directions.

Keywords: Mental health, Time demands, NCAA, Student-athletes, Well-being, Academic time use, Athletic time use.



1. INTRODUCTION

Obtaining a college degree, being a priority of any college student, is academic in nature. While this also holds true for student-athletes, there are athletic expectations that come into play. The rigor of routine practices and other athletic activities while attending regular classes and complying with course requirements adds to the typical challenges of intercollegiate athletics. As such, it can be argued that because they deal with time constraints in meeting their academic and athletic responsibilities, the state of their well-being can be compromised. While the outcomes of the academic time commitment of the general college student population have been well-explored in the academic literature, the state of research on student-athletes’ well-being as a consequence of the challenges that arise from academic and athletic time use needs to be investigated.

Previous studies have established that subjective well-being is improved by engagement in physical activity, including sports [1-5]. However, the results of studies on the subjective well-being of student-athletes, in general, are mixed and seemingly contradicting. In fact, it was previously argued that sports can be detrimental or beneficial to the well-being of athletes [6]. As some research claimed that sports participation alone promotes positive well-being, a past study investigated the role of sports involvement on subjective well-being and put forward that playing sports can indeed influence subjective well-being [7]. It was posited that adults who play sports on a regular basis rate their happiness higher than non-players. Meanwhile, another study compared the subjective well-being of active and retired elite athletes and found that the subjective well-being index of active athletes is higher than those of retired athletes [8]. The findings demonstrate that retired athletes have lower subjective well-being a year after retirement than athletes who are still active in competitions.

On the contrary, some studies highlighted a negative impact of sports participation on well-being. The life satisfaction of elite athletes in Australia was assessed for a period of five years, and it was determined that current athletes have lower life satisfaction than their retired peers [9]. Further, research suggests that athletic injury is not only one of the primary reasons for involuntary retirement from sports [10] but also a significant predictor of decreased subjective well-being. In this premise, a previous study concluded that perceived stress is associated with diminished life satisfaction of injured student-athletes, and injured student-athletes who report higher stress and little social support are more likely to experience lower subjective well-being [11].

Several factors have been identified as predictors of subjective well-being in the context of sports. In a systematic review of subjective well-being studies in the context of sports performance, the common predictors identified were gratitude, mindfulness, character strengths, coaching, hope, and spirituality [12]. A number of studies have also explored other socio-demographic factors such as gender, with male athletes having a higher level of life satisfaction [13], and race, with African American college student-athletes having a lower level of life satisfaction than their White counterparts [14].

Van Rens and colleagues [15] investigated the link between student-athlete identities and well-being. Their findings revealed that student-athletes’ academic identity is significantly correlated with life satisfaction. In addition, Surujlal et al. [16] ascertained that subjective well-being positively correlates with coping skills, while the same variable is negatively correlated with perceived stress. Moreover, Galli and Reel [17] examined the adversities experienced by student-athletes from three universities in the western part of the United States. The top three adversities identified by the respondents were time demands, physical stress, and mental stress.

Evidence of the benefits of physical activities such as sport and exercise is established. For instance, Salmon [18] noted that physical exercise acted as a buffer against anxiety, depression, and stress. Another benefit is emotional well-being, which has been claimed to facilitate positive development among adolescents [19]. The positive impact of physical activity, including sport, on emotional well-being encompasses different populations, such as middle-aged women [20], adults [21], adolescents [22-24], children [25, 26], and college students [27, 28].

Several studies have dealt with the impact of social and personal factors on emotional well-being. For instance, appraisal of threat/challenge was found to be significantly associated with the emotional well-being of club-level athletes, although it is important to note that the mediation effect, and not a direct effect, was tested [29]. This indicates that a higher level of emotional well-being is experienced by athletes who treat competition as an opportunity for growth and mastery of skills. In another study, it was concluded that perceived autonomy support, autonomy, and intrinsic goal motivation were associated with emotional well-being among Olympic athletes [30], which indicates that athletes who feel supported and are motivated by their own goals have higher emotional well-being. Conversely, emotional well-being is positively predicted by peer task-involving climate but negatively predicted by coach ego-involving climate in research among youth athletes [31]. It is clear in their findings that emotional well-being is influenced by the type of sports environment athletes participate in.

While research on emotional well-being in general is extensive, literature specific to student-athletes is sparse. Some clues, however, can be derived from related studies. Brunstein et al. [32] examined the relationship between personal goals and the emotional well-being of college students. They found that emotional well-being is positively predicted by the interaction of goal commitment and goal attainability of motive-congruent goals. In addition, high commitment to motive-incongruent goals predicted a decrease in emotional well-being. These results can serve as a foundation for determining the effects of student-athletes’ time use on their emotional well-being. According to the NCAA [33], there are more student-athletes who intend to graduate from college than to pursue a career in professional sports. This finding is supported by the NCAA GOALS data, which indicated that student-athletes spend more time in academics rather than athletics [34]. The goals of student-athletes are more educational than athletic, and thus, it can be argued that time used in academics will positively predict higher emotional well-being while time used in athletics will negatively predict emotional well-being.

On positioning for success in intercollegiate athletics, one has to be physically prepared. Contrastingly however, Wiese-Bjornstal [35] believes that the most effective means of being competitive in college athletics can also be destructive to physical health. The author pointed out that sports injury can be debilitating to student-athletes, given that athletes are prone to the excessive effort to the point of overtraining. Aside from the risks involved in playing, athletes can also get injured in training and practice. The tendency for them to glorify pain and suffering [36] only adds to these risks.

Accordingly, there is the contention that athletic participation can be detrimental to physical health. The risks are notably high and the chance of being seriously injured becomes akin to playing the sport itself. Once injured, it can be assumed that subjective and emotional well-being suffer, too, as reported in various studies [11, 37, 38]. Interestingly, studies on the direct link between time use and the physical well-being of student-athletes are virtually scarce. As such, the need to study the impact of time use on physical well-being has been warranted. Further, research on student-athletes’ well-being has not directly explored its connection to time utilization. The need for more studies on the life satisfaction of young adults, where most college student-athletes belong, has been echoed [39]. Studies on subjective well-being specific to the group of student-athletes are necessary given their unique context and the research evidence that showed the role of culture on subjective well-being [40]. Fundamentally, context can influence subjective well-being, as shown in the significantly different levels of life satisfaction among Korean and American adolescents [41]. Thus, the current study examined the effects of academic time use and athletic time use on subjective well-being, physical well-being, and emotional well-being using a large representative sample of NCAA student-athletes. The following research questions were investigated:

RQ1: To what extent does academic time use predict subjective emotional and physical well-being?

RQ2: To what extent does athletic time use predict subjective, emotional, and physical well-being?

It is hypothesized that academic time use would be a significant positive predictor of subjective, emotional, and physical well-being, while athletic time use would negatively predict the three well-being dimensions.

2. MATERIALS AND METHODS

2.1. Participants

The sample is composed of 5,042 respondents from the NCAA GOALS 2015 dataset, which was provided by the NCAA Research through a data use agreement. The approval of the university’s Institutional Review Board was sought prior to data release. The authors were given access to a sample of 21,219 respondents. Random sampling was employed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS). Most of the respondents are men (n = 2,752; 54.6%) and White (n = 3,634; 72.1%). There are almost the same number of research participants per division level: Division I = 1,720 (34.1%), Division II = 1,517 (30.1%), and Division III = 1,805 (35.8%).

2.2. Measures

Academic time use and athletic time use are the independent variables. Academic time use includes the number of hours per week spent on attending classes and doing class-related activities such as study halls and tutorials. Athletic time use pertains to the number of hours spent per week on athletic activities such as training, games, and travel to game sites. The dependent measures include the 3-item physical well-being, 2-item emotional well-being, and 3-item subjective well-being. These measures were developed by the NCAA through their GOALS research program and validated in succeeding research [42]. Higher scores indicate a higher level of well-being.

The items for subjective well-being included “In the last month, how often have you felt that things were going your way?”, “In the last month, how often have you felt about your ability to handle your personal problems?”, and “All things considered, how happy are you today?” This measure has a Cronbach’s alpha of .64. Participants answered these items using a 5-point scale (1 = very often; 2 = fairly often; 3 = sometimes; 4 = almost never; 5 = never).

“In the last month, how often have you felt difficulties were piling up so high that you could not overcome them?” and “In the last month, how often have you felt that you were unable to control the important things in your life?” were the items for emotional well-being. The Cronbach’s alpha attached to this measure is .77. Participants also answered both items using a 5-point scale (1 = very often; 2 = fairly often; 3 = sometimes; 4 = almost never; 5 = never).

The items, “During the last 30 days, how many days did you have a headache?”, “During the last 30 days, how many days did you have a cold, flu or similar illness?”, and “During the last 30 days, how many days did you have trouble sleeping?” captured physical well-being (PWB). The items were reported using a frequency scale that comprised the following categories of 15+ days, 8-14 days, 4-7 days, 1-3 days, and none. The Cronbach’s alpha is measured at .58.

2.3. Data Analysis

A series of two-step hierarchical regression analyses were performed to answer the two research questions. Before conducting the regression analysis, gender was dummy coded as 1 for men and 0 for women, the race was dummy coded as 1 for Whites and 0 for student-athletes of color, and division level was dummy coded as 1 for Division I and 0 for Divisions II and III. In step 1, race, division level, and gender as control variables were loaded in block 1. Step 2 placed a predictor variable (academic time use or athletic time use) in block 2. For each research question, three separate two-step hierarchical regressions predicting each of the well-being indicators (subjective well-being, emotional well-being, and physical well-being) were conducted. For RQ1, academic time use was used as a predictor of subjective well-being, emotional well-being, and physical well-being in different regression models. Similarly, for RQ2 athletic time use served as the independent variable for each of the three well-being indicators in separate regression analyses. The R2 for step 1, ∆ R2 for step 2, the total R2, and the standardized beta (β) values are reported below. The significance level was determined at p < .05.

3. RESULTS

3.1. Academic Time Use

The results for RQ1 and RQ2 are summarized in two regression analysis tables. Table 1 shows the extent to which athletic time use predicts the three indicators of well-being. For effect on subjective well-being, results of Step 1 showed that the control variables accounted for .002% of the variance in the dependent variable, F(3, 5038) = 3.373, p = .018. Within block 1, race, β = -.033, p = .019 and gender, β = -.034, p = .016 were found to be statistically significant covariates in the model, but division level was not significant, β = -.008, p = .571. Adding academic time use in Step 2 indicated a significant .1% variance increase, β = .033, p = .023, ∆R2 = .001, and the model remains significant, F(4, 5037) = 3.825, p = .004.

For effect on emotional well-being, results in Step 1 revealed that the demographic variables significantly contributed to 1.3% of the variance, F(3, 5038) = 22.134, p < .001. Race, β = .068, p < .001, and gender, β = .100, p < .001, are statistically significant, but division level is not significant, β = -.008, p = .559. When academic time use was added in Step 2, the variance did not change, β = -.023, p = .114, ∆R2 < .001, although the model remains significant, F(4, 5037) = 17.365, p < .001. For effect on physical well-being, results in Step 1 showed that the demographic variables significantly explained 1.5% of the variance, F(3, 5038) = 25.862, p < .001. Division level, β = -.033, p = .018, and gender, β = .119, p < .001, are statistically significant, but race is not significant, β = .002, p = .866. The variance did not change after adding academic time use to the model, β = -.004, p = .788, ∆R2 = .000, but the model remains significant, F(4, 5037) = 19.411, p < .001.

The extent to which academic time use predicts the three well-being indicators is shown in Table 2.

Table 1. Summary of the regression analyses for academic time use as predictor of subjective well-being, emotional well-being, and physical well-being.
- F R2 ∆R2 B SE β
Criterion variable: subjective well-being
Step 1 3.373* .002
Division level -.009 .016 -.008
Race -.039 .017 -.033*
Gender -.036 .015 -.034*
Step 2 3.825** .003 .001
Division level -.006 .016 -.005
Race -.037 .017 -.031*
Gender -.031 .015 -.029*
Academic time use .001 .000 .033*
Criterion variable: emotional well-being
Step 1 22.134*** .013
Division level -.018 .031 -.008
Race .157 .033 .068***
Gender .208 .029 .100***
Step 2 17.365*** .013 .000
Division level -.022 .031 -.010
Race .153 .033 .067***
Gender .200 .030 .097***
Academic time use -.001 .001 -.023
Criterion variable: physical well-being
Step 1 25.862*** .015
Division level -.060 .025 -.033*
Race .005 .027 .002
Gender .205 .024 .119***
Step 2 (2-way interaction) 19.411*** .015 .000
Division level -.061 .026 -.034*
Race .004 .027 .002
Gender .204 .025 .118***
Academic time use .000 .001 -.004
*** p < .001
** p < .01
* p < .05
Table 2. Summary of the regression analyses for academic time use as predictor of subjective well-being, emotional well-being, and physical well-being.
- F R2 ∆R2 B SE β
Criterion variable: subjective well-being
Step 1 3.373* .002
Division level -.009 .016 -.008
Race -.039 .017 -.033*
Gender -.036 .015 -.034*
Step 2 4.670 ** .004 .002
Division level -.012 .016 -.011
Race -.036 .017 -.031*
Gender -.039 .015 -.037**
Athletic time use .001 .000 .041**
Criterion variable: emotional well-being
Step 1 22.134*** .013
Division level -.018 .031 -.008
Race .157 .033 .068***
Gender .208 .029 .100***
Step 2 20.469*** .016 .003
Division level -.010 .031 -.005
Race .149 .033 .065***
Gender .216 .029 .105***
Athletic time use -.004 .001 -.054***
Criterion variable: physical well-being
Step 1 25.862*** .015
Division level -.060 .025 -.033*
Race .005 .027 .002
Gender .205 .024 .119***
Step 2 (2-way interaction) 23.641*** .018 .003
Division level -.054 .025 -.030*
Race -.002 .027 -.001
Gender .213 .024 .123***
Athletic time use -.003 .001 -.058***
*** p < .001
** p < .01
* p < .05

3.2. Athletic Time Use

For the effect on subjective well-being, adding athletic time use to the model explained a significant .2% variance increase, β = .041, p = .003, ∆R2 = .002, and the model remains significant, F(4, 5037) = 4.670, p = .001. For the effect on emotional well-being, adding athletic time use to the model explained a significant variance increase of .3%, β = -.054, p < .001, ∆R2 = .003. The model after Step 2 remains significant, F(4, 5037) = 20.469, p < .001. For the effect on physical well-being, adding athletic time use to the model increased the variance by .3%, β = -.058, p < .001, ∆R2 = .003. The model after Step 2 continues to be significant F(4, 5037) = 23.641, p < .001.

4. DISCUSSION

This study investigated the consequences of time commitments on the well-being of college student-athletes. Significant negative predictive value of athletic time utilization on emotional well-being and physical well-being was observed. The negative effect of athletic time use on physical well-being (PWB) is aptly supported in the extant literature. Training and practices take a physical toll on the players’ bodies. It can be put forward that physical exhaustion is one of the issues reported by student-athletes in the survey that determined the impact of time demands [43]. Moreover, there are innate risks associated with playing sports. It is given that playing high-level competitive sports, such as intercollegiate athletics, is physically-taxing. Preparation for athletic competition makes athletes susceptible to overtraining and injuries [35]. Considering also that athletes are known to take pain and suffering with glory [36], it can be posited that there is a higher than usual tendency to overexert themselves physically. Combined with the enormous time demands on athletics, student-athletes can be expected to experience a decreased level of physical well-being. These findings, therefore, imply that the more time student-athletes spend on athletics, the lower their physical well-being becomes.

The finding that athletic time use negatively predicts emotional well-being can be explained by the dynamics of the relationship between emotional well-being and personal goals. Previous research has established that among college students, emotional well-being is positively predicted by the interaction of goal commitment and goal attainability of motive-congruent goals but negatively predicted by a high commitment to motive-incongruent goals [32]. The foremost goal of any college student is towards an eventual degree. For student-athletes, however, there is an opportunity for a professional sports career after their college playing years are over. In fact, according to NCAA [33], student-athletes indeed have strong athletic expectations, which suggests that academics may not be their priority. However, universities exist for academic reasons, and research has shown that student-athletes are reminded of the importance of having academic and athletic balance in the landscape [44]; hence, most student-athletes prefer having a college degree [33]. Therefore, it is asserted that spending too much time on athletics does not help the commonly-accepted purpose of attending college getting a college degree because any time spent outside academics can compromise the advancement towards that goal. In addition, academic time use facilitates better quality of life in college [45], which further suggests that time spent on other aspects of college life, especially the magnitude of time student-athletes spend on athletics, is detrimental to well-being. Hence, the negative effect of athletic time utilization on emotional well-being.

Athletic time use has a positive effect on subjective well-being, as indicated by the derived values. This offers a fresh perspective to reexamine how athletic time must be framed. This finding, being counterintuitive, thus opens an area for further investigation. One possible explanation for this is that sport is seen as a form of escape for student-athletes and away from their foremost tasks and responsibilities in academics as students. In arguing that sport serves as an escape from the challenges of everyday life, Segrave [46] stated, “At worst, sport is not so much an escape from life but an inversion of it first, and ultimately evasion altogether” (p. 76). As such, time devoted to athletics can have a cathartic effect. Previous research has found that stress and sports fandom are positively correlated [47], indicating that the utility of athletics in subjective well-being goes beyond the athletes themselves.

The results demonstrated that academic time use positively predicts subjective well-being. This finding is consistent with the results of a previous study that the time spent in academics is a significant predictor in the quality of life of college students [45]. This suggests that because college students, student-athletes included, are in school primarily for a degree, spending time to that end and consequently getting related tasks to make them feel better about themselves. In addition, time spent in academics suggests decisiveness in their career goals. It can be inferred from these findings that student-athletes who prioritize academics over athletics seem to understand that a bachelor’s degree is more valuable. Past studies suggested that career-decidedness is a significant predictor of subjective well-being among college students [48].

CONCLUSION

Both time utilizations in academics and athletics are significant predictors of subjective well-being. Athletic time use negatively predicts emotional well-being and physical well-being, indicating the toll athletics have on well-being. While significant findings are in place, caution must be observed when considering the standards of Cohen [49] in interpreting the value of variance explained in multiple regression. None of the ∆R2 values is higher than the 2% cut-off for small effect size. Some limitations of the study must also be emphasized. First, our study did not test for covariates. Second, the psychometric properties could be improved with further iterations. Beyond these, however, our findings open up some potential areas and topics for succeeding research to advance the knowledge and practice aligned with the variables of interest examined in this study.

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS

SPSS = Statistical Package for Social Sciences
PWB = Physical Well-Being

ETHICS APPROVAL AND CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE

This study was approved by University of Florida’s Institutional Review Board.

HUMAN AND ANIMAL RIGHTS

No animals were used for studies that are the basis of this research. All the humans used were in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975.

CONSENT FOR PUBLICATION

Participants signed off a consent form to participate in the original survey.

STANDARDS OF REPORTING

STROBE guidelines were followed.

AVAILABILITY OF DATA AND MATERIALS

The dataset utilized in this study is protected by a data use agreement. Subsequent use must be requested from the NCAA Research.

FUNDING

None.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors declare no conflict of interest, financial or otherwise.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We are thankful to the NCAA Research, especially to Keke Liu and Lydia Bell.

REFERENCES

[1] Berger BG. Subjective well-being in obese individuals: The multiple roles of exercise. Quest 2004; 56(1): 50-76.
[2] Carels RA, Berger B, Darby L. The association between mood states and physical activity in postmenopausal, obese, sedentary women. J Aging Phys Act 2006; 14(1): 12-28.
[3] Ekkekakis P, Hall EE, VanLanduyt LM, Petruzzello SJ. Walking in (affective) circles: can short walks enhance affect? J Behav Med 2000; 23(3): 245-75.
[4] Lane AM, Jackson A, Terry PC. Preferred modality influences on exercise-induced mood changes. J Sports Sci Med 2005; 4(2): 195-200.
[5] Rocheleau CA, Webster GD, Bryan A, Frazier J. Moderators of the relationship between exercise and mood changes: gender, exertion level, and workout duration. Psychol Health 2004; 19(4): 491-506.
[6] Bartholomew KJ, Ntoumanis N, Ryan RM, Bosch JA, Thøgersen-Ntoumani C. Self-determination theory and diminished functioning: the role of interpersonal control and psychological need thwarting. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2011; 37(11): 1459-73.
[7] Forrest D, McHale IG. Subjective well-being and engagement in sport: Evidence from England.The economics of sport, health and happiness: The promotion of well-being through sporting activities 2011; 186-99.
[8] Stephan Y, Bilard J, Ninot G, Delignieres D. Repercussions of transition out of elite sport on subjective well-being: A one-year study. J Appl Sport Psychol 2003; 15(4): 354-71.
[9] Martin LA, Fogarty GJ, Albion MJ. Changes in athletic identity and life satisfaction of elite athletes as a function of retirement status. J Appl Sport Psychol 2014; 26(1): 96-110.
[10] Stambulova NB. Athlete’s crises: A developmental perspective. Int J Sport Psychol 2000; 31(4): 584-601.
[11] Malinauskas R. The associations among social support, stress, and life satisfaction as perceived by injured college athletes. Soc Behav Personal 2010; 38(6): 741-52.
[12] Donaldson SI, Dollwet M, Rao MA. Happiness, excellence, and optimal human functioning revisited: Examining the peer-reviewed literature linked to positive psychology. J Posit Psychol 2015; 10(3): 185-95.
[13] Varca PE, Shaffer GS, Saunders V. A Longitudinal Investigation of Sport Participation and Life Satisfaction. J Sport Psychol 1984; 6(4): 440-7.
[14] Perna FM, Ahlgren RL, Zaichkowsky L. The influence of career planning, race, and athletic injury on life satisfaction among recently retired collegiate male athletes. Sport Psychol 1999; 13(2): 144-56.
[15] van Rens FECA, Ashley RA, Steele AR. Well-Being and performance in dual careers: The role of academic and athletic identities. Sport Psychol 2019; 33(1): 42-51.
[16] Surujlal J, Van Zyl Y, Nolan VT. Perceived stress and coping skills of university student-athletes and the relationship with life satisfaction. African Journal for Physical Health Education. Recreation and Dance 2013; 19(4): 1047-59.
[17] Galli N, Reel JJ. Can good come from bad? An examination of adversarial growth in Division I NCAA athletes. J Intercoll Sport 2012; 5(2): 199-212.
[18] Salmon P. Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression, and sensitivity to stress. Clin Psychol Rev 2001; 21(1): 33-61.
[19] Park N. The role of subjective well-being in positive youth development. Ann Am Acad Pol Soc Sci 2004; 591(1): 25-39.
[20] Lee C, Russell A. Effects of physical activity on emotional well-being among older Australian women. J Psychosom Res 2003; 54(2): 155-60.
[21] Meyer TJ, Mark MM. Effects of psychosocial interventions with adult cancer patients: A meta-analysis of randomized experiments. Health Psychol 1995; 14(2): 101-8.
[22] Lubans DR, Plotnikoff RC, Lubans NJ. Review: A systematic review of the impact of physical activity programmes on social and emotional well-being in at-risk youth. Child Adolesc Ment Health 2012; 17(1): 2-13.
[23] Spruit A, Assink M, van Vugt E, van der Put C, Stams GJ. The effects of physical activity interventions on psychosocial outcomes in adolescents: A meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 2016; 45: 56-71.
[24] Steptoe AS, Butler N. Sports participation and emotional wellbeing in adolescents. Lancet 1996; 347(9018): 1789-92.
[25] Parfitt G, Eston RG. The relationship between children’s habitual activity level and psychological well-being. Acta Paediatr 2005; 94(12): 1791-7.
[26] Tkacz J, Young-Hyman D, Boyle CA, Davis CL. Aerobic exercise program reduces anger expression among overweight children. Pediatr Exerc Sci 2008; 20(4): 390-401.
[27] Giacobbi PR, Hausenblas HA, Frye N. A naturalistic assessment of the relationship between personality, daily life events, leisure-time exercise, and mood. Psychol Sport Exerc 2005; 6(1): 67-81.
[28] Giacobbi PR Jr, Tuccitto DE, Frye N. Exercise, affect, and university students’ appraisals of academic events prior to the final examination period. Psychol Sport Exerc 2007; 8(2): 261-74.
[29] Adie JW, Duda JL, Ntoumanis N. Achievement goals, competition appraisals, and the psychological and emotional welfare of sport participants. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2008; 30(3): 302-22.
[30] Solberg PA, Halvari H. Perceived autonomy support, personal goal content, and emotional well-being among elite athletes: mediating effects of reasons for goals. Percept Mot Skills 2009; 108(3): 721-43.
[31] Ntoumanis N, Taylor IM, Thøgersen-Ntoumani C. A longitudinal examination of coach and peer motivational climates in youth sport: Implications for moral attitudes, well-being, and behavioral investment. Dev Psychol 2012; 48(1): 213-23.
[32] Brunstein JC, Schultheiss OC, Grässman R. Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. J Pers Soc Psychol 1998; 75(2): 494-508.
[33] National Collegiate Athletic Association. 2017.NCAA GOALS and SCORE studies http://www.ncaa.org/sites/default/files/2017GOALS_Full_Report_20170628.pdf b
[34] National Collegiate Athletic Association. Results from the 2015 GOALS study of the student-athlete experience.NCAA 2016 Convention Indianapolis: NCAA 2016.
[35] Wiese-Bjornstal DM. Sport injury and college athlete health across the lifespan. J Intercoll Sport 2009; 2(1): 64-80.
[36] Coakley JJ. Sport in society: Issues and controversies 6th ed. 1998.
[37] Kleiber DA, Brock SC. The effect of career-ending injuries on the subsequent well-being of elite college athletes. Sociol Sport J 1992; 9(1): 70-5.
[38] Moreira NB, Vagetti GC, de Oliveira V, de Campos W. Association between injury and quality of life in athletes: A systematic review, 1980–2013. Apunts Medicina de l’Esport 2014; 49(184): 123-38.
[39] Huebner ES, Funk BA III, Gilman R. Cross-sectional and longitudinal psychosocial correlates of adolescent life satisfaction reports. Can J Sch Psychol 2000; 16(1): 53-64.
[40] Diener E, Ryan K. Subjective well-being: A general overview. S Afr J Psychol 2009; 39(4): 391-406.
[41] Park N, Huebner ES. A cross-cultural study of the levels and correlates of life satisfaction among adolescents. J Cross Cult Psychol 2005; 36(4): 444-56.
[42] Santos JC, Sagas M. Differential effects of sport type and sport category on time demands and well-being of college athletes. International Journal of Human Movement and Sports Sciences 2022; 10(1): 22-30.
[43] Pacific Coast Conference 12. (2016). PAC-12 report on student-athlete time demands. 2016.http://static.pac-12.com/PAC12TimeDemands160512.pdf
[44] Jayakumar UM, Comeaux E. The cultural cover-up of college athletics: How organizational culture perpetuates an unrealistic and idealized balancing act. J High Educ (Columb Ohio) 2016; 87(4): 488-515.
[45] Greene KM, Maggs JL. Academic time during college: Associations with mood, tiredness, and binge drinking across days and semesters. J Adolesc 2017; 56(1): 24-33.
[46] Segrave JO. Sport as Escape. J Sport Soc Issues 2000; 24(1): 61-77.
[47] Wann DL, Allen B, Rochelle AR. Using sport fandom as an escape: Searching for relief from under-stimulation and over-stimulation. International Sports Journal 2004; 8(1): 104-13.
[48] Uthayakumar R, Schimmack U, Hartung PJ, Rogers JR. Career decidedness as a predictor of SWB. J Vocat Behav 2010; 77(2): 196-204.
[49] Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Jolly, J. C. (2008). Raising the question# 9 is the student-athlete population unique? And why should we care? Communication Education 1988; 57(1): 145-51.